Skip to main content

Further evidence that CP-AMPARs are critically involved in synaptic tag and capture at hippocampal CA1 synapses

Abstract

The synaptic tag and capture (STC) hypothesis provides an important theoretical basis for understanding the synaptic basis of associative learning. We recently provided pharmacological evidence that calcium-permeable AMPA receptors (CP-AMPARs) are a crucial component of this form of heterosynaptic metaplasticity. Here we have investigated two predictions that arise on the basis of CP-AMPARs serving as a trigger of STC. Firstly, we compared the effects of the order in which we delivered a strong theta burst stimulation (TBS) protocol (75 pulses) and a weak TBS protocol (15 pulses) to two independent inputs. We only observed significant heterosynaptic metaplasticity when the strong TBS preceded the weak TBS. Second, we found that pausing stimulation following either the sTBS or the wTBS for ~20 min largely eliminates the heterosynaptic metaplasticity. These observations are consistent with a process that is triggered by the synaptic insertion of CP-AMPARs and provide a framework for establishing the underlying molecular mechanisms.

Introduction

The concept of synaptic tag and capture (STC) was introduced to explain how input-specificity of synaptic plasticity could be maintained in the presence of de novo protein synthesis. In the initial pioneering experiments it was shown that a strong induction protocol to one input not only generated a protein synthesis-dependent form of long-term potentiation (LTP) at that input but was able to enable a subsequent weak induction protocol at an independent input to generate a larger and more sustained LTP [8]. The original proposed mechanism is that a sufficiently strong induction protocol initiates cell-wide protein synthesis to generate plasticity-related proteins (PRPs) and that the weak protocol sets a synaptic tag that enables these locally-tagged synapses to capture PRPs. Since the discovery of this STC phenomenon there has been a massive effort to understand the underlying molecular mechanisms that are responsible for the formation of PRPs and the identity of the synaptic tag, given the relevance of this synaptic process to associative learning and memory [22].

Previously, we showed that calcium-permeable α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid receptors (CP-AMPARs) are transiently expressed during some forms of LTP at Schaffer collateral-commissural synapses [16, 20] and proposed that these could be involved in the STC process. Recently, we provided direct evidence that this was indeed the case. We found that if we pharmacologically inhibited CP-AMPARs, during and shortly following the strong induction protocol, then the facilitation of LTP in response to the weak induction protocol was eliminated [18]. Therefore, we proposed that the synaptic activation of CP-AMPARs is involved in the triggering of PRPs, and that these newly synthesized proteins are engaged by the weak induction protocol to facilitate the LTP on the independent input. We also found that inhibiting CP-AMPARs during and following just the weak induction protocol resulted in a partial inhibition of the facilitation of LTP. We proposed therefore that CP-AMPARs are also required during induction of LTP by the weak input for the full heterosynaptic metaplastic effect to be observed.

This mechanism leads to two predications, that we have tested in the present study using theta burst stimulation (TBS) protocols. First, this CP-AMPAR-based mechanism requires the strong induction protocol to precede the weak one, since multiple trains are required to drive CP-AMPARs to the synapse under the recording conditions of our experiments. Second, on the assumption that once inserted CP-AMPARs need to be activated synaptically to elicit their effect, then stopping stimulation should mimic the effects of pharmacological inhibition of CP-AMPARs, in terms of the alterations in synaptic strength. To test these predications we interleaved four sets of experiments where the weak induction protocol comprised a single episode of TBS, termed weak TBS (wTBS; 15 stimuli), and the strong induction protocol comprised three episodes of TBS that were spaced in time with an inter-episode interval of 10 min, termed spaced TBS (sTBS; 75 stimuli). For set A, we delivered the wTBS 30 min after the sTBS, to replicate the control experiments reported previously [18]. For set B, we reversed the order of presentation of the sTBS and wTBS. For sets C and D, we reverted to sTBS before wTBS but stopped stimulation of both inputs immediately following either the sTBS (set C) or the wTBS (set D). Our observations are entirely consistent with the notion that CP-AMPARs are inserted as a result of the sTBS to enable enhanced LTP to be induced on an independent input by the wTBS. They are also consistent with the idea that CP-AMPARs are additionally engaged by the wTBS to contribute to the induction of the facilitated LTP. Therefore CP-AMPARs are crucial for this form of heterosynaptic metaplasticity. They do so by initiating a heterosynaptic priming effect, where they both locally trigger de novo protein synthesis and tag inputs for heterosynaptic metaplasticity.

Results

To compare the effects of the order of presentation of the weak and strong induction protocols we used two protocols:

For set A, we delivered the sTBS 30 min before the wTBS was delivered to an independent input. As described previously for a different data set, this greatly facilitated the magnitude of LTP induced by the wTBS (Fig. 1), via a process that we refer to as heterosynaptic priming [18]. For example, the level of the LTP induced by a wTBS (averaged over a 1 min period, 90 min following TBS) was 124 ± 4%, n = 8 (Fig. 1a, d) and 156 ± 7%, n = 11 (Fig. 1b, d) in the absence or presence of heterosynaptic priming (p = 0.0023, one-way ANOVA with Bonferroni’s post hoc test, F(2, 24) = 7.76). As noted previously [18], the LTP induced by a sTBS, but not that induced by a wTBS, was also associated with a small heterosynaptic LTP (Fig. 1a, b).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Evidence that wTBS before sTBS does not effectively trigger heterosynaptic metaplasticity. a Two-input experiment that shows that a wTBS (15 pulses) induces a small, persistent input-specific LTP (n = 8). In this and subsequent time-course plots the data are mean ± SEM and representative traces show the superimposition of baseline and potentiated fEPSPs (averages of 5 successive records at the times indicated by numbers). b Delivery of a sTBS to one input (filled circles) resulted in a small heterosynaptic LTP in the second input plus an enhanced LTP induced by a wTBS (open symbols; n = 11). c Delivery of a wTBS to one input (open circles) resulted in a small LTP that was not appreciably affected by a subsequent sTBS on the other input (n = 8). d Superimposition of the LTP induced by the wTBS illustrated in panels a and b. e Superimposition of the LTP induced by the wTBS illustrated in panels a and c. f Quantification of the level of LTP, 90 min post wTBS (wLTP) under conditions of no priming (grey bar and data points), sTBS before wTBS (S→W) (white) and wTBS before sTBS (W→S) (brown). The bar graphs present the mean data ± SEM (**p < 0.01) and the cumulative distribution plots present each individual result along with a dashed line to show each group mean

For set B, we reversed the order of presentation of the TBS (Fig. 1c, e). We found that the level of LTP induced by the wTBS was not significantly different whether a heterosynaptic priming stimulus was delivered (132 ± 7%, n = 8) or not (124 ± 4%, n = 8, p = 0.7370, one-way ANOVA with Bonferroni’s post hoc test). We noted a trend for an enhanced response (also see summary plots in Fig. 1f), but this effect could be attributed to the small heterosynaptic LTP induced by the sTBS.

To test the effects of stopping stimulation we compared two additional protocols (Fig. 2). For set C, we stopped stimulation for 20 min following the third TBS episode (Fig. 2a, c); in addition, during the sTBS protocol we also stopped stimulation for most of the time following the second TBS episode. When compared to the experiments of set A, we found that the level of LTP induced by the wTBS (118 ± 6%, n = 9) was effectively identical to that observed following the wTBS in the absence of heterosynaptic priming (Fig. 1d, 2f; p = 0.9716, one-way ANOVA with Bonferroni’s post hoc test) and was significantly less than that when heterosynaptic priming was employed (p = 0.0002, one-way ANOVA with Bonferroni’s post hoc test, F(2,25) = 12.13). Thus, interrupting stimulation during and following the sTBS completely prevented the STC process from occurring.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Evidence that stopping stimulation impairs heterosynaptic metaplasticity. a An experiment performed as in Fig. 1b, except that stimulation was paused on both inputs for 10 min following the 2nd TBS and 20 min following the 3rd TBS (n = 9). b An experiment performed as in Fig. 1b, except that stimulation on both inputs was paused for 20 min following the wTBS (n = 12). c Superimposition of the LTP induced by the wTBS illustrated in a, Fig. 1a, b. d Superimposition of the LTP induced by the wTBS illustrated in b, Fig. 1a, b. e Quantification of the level of LTP induced by the sTBS for these experiments, 120 min post sTBS. f Quantification of the level of LTP induced by the wTBS for these experiments, 90 min post wTBS. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, comparisons vs. sTBS

For set D, we stopped stimulation for 20 min following the wTBS episode. We found that the level of LTP induced by the wTBS following heterosynaptic priming (135 ± 5 %, n = 12) was significantly less than that observed when there was no pause in stimulation following the wTBS (Fig. 1b, d, 2f; p = 0.0215, one-way ANOVA with Bonferroni’s post hoc test) and not significantly different from that observed in the absence of heterosynaptic priming (Fig. 1a, d, 2f; p = 0.4539, one-way ANOVA with Bonferroni’s post hoc test, F(2,28) = 7.39). The trend for enhanced LTP in this post-wTBS stop experiment could be explained by the small heterosynaptic potentiation. Therefore, interrupting stimulation following the wTBS also substantially reduced, if not abolished, the STC effect.

Discussion

In the present study we have extended our work that has investigated the hypothesis that CP-AMPARs are important for the STC effect [1820]. We performed four interleaved sets of experiments (A–D) that support the notion that CP-AMPARs are critically involved in this process.

In set A, we confirmed our own observation, with an entirely new data set, that a wTBS delivered 30 min after a sTBS on an independent input leads to a greatly enhanced LTP compared to that induced by a wTBS alone. This result is fully in agreement with the original STC experiments of Frey and Morris [8]. Previously we showed that it is the timing rather than the strength per se of the “strong” TBS that is critical for the STC effect [18]. We compared two protocols, each three episodes of theta (75 pulses in total) where the inter-episodes were either 10 s (compressed) or 10 min (spaced) and observed the STC effect only with the spaced protocol. Other work had shown that compressed and spaced LTP induction protocols differ in that the latter specifically engages a component of LTP that requires activation of PKA and de novo protein synthesis (e.g., [11]) and CP-AMPARs [17]. In the present study we therefore used our standard spaced TBS protocol as the “strong” stimulus.

Since CP-AMPARs are only transiently inserted into these synapses, with a dwell time in the order of minutes [19], we previously hypothesized [20] and then demonstrated [18] that these receptors could serve to “tag” synapses for enhanced LTP. Our data support a model whereby the sTBS initiates local de novo protein synthesis by leading to the transient insertion of CP-AMPARs in a two-step process that requires, firstly, the insertion of these receptors at perisynaptic sites and, secondly, their movement into the synapse [17]. The first TBS in the episode solely engages the first step whereas the subsequent TBS episodes drives these newly plasma membrane inserted CP-AMPARs into the synapse. This mechanism can fully account for why spaced protocols are required to generate the protein synthesis-dependent component of LTP, that is commonly referred to as LTP2 [3] or late-phase LTP [11]. Since the sTBS enables enhanced LTP at a heterosynaptic input, as defined by the lack of heterosynaptic paired-pulse facilitation, we referred to the process as heterosynaptic priming [18], which is a form of heterosynaptic metaplasticity [12].

The order of presentation of “strong” and “weak” induction protocols

In set B, we examined the effects of delivering the wTBS before the sTBS. According to the original STC hypothesis this should also be effective, though less so than delivery of the sTBS beforehand (see [8, 9]). However, according to our CP-AMPAR mechanism, the wTBS before the sTBS should not be effective. This is because a single episode of TBS, as in our wTBS protocol, would not be expected to drive CP-AMPARs into the synapse, under the conditions of our experiments. Consistent with our hypothesis we did not observe a significant STC effect when we delivered the wTBS before the sTBS. We did, however, observe a trend for a facilitation of the subsequent LTP but we believe that this can be accounted for by the small heterosynaptic LTP that accompanies LTP induced by a sTBS [8, 18]. Therefore, we can conclude that to establish this form of heterosynaptic metaplasticity the “strong” stimulus must precede the “weak” stimulus under our experimental conditions. Of course, this doesn’t discount the weak before strong protocol being effective under different circumstances. Although a single episode of TBS does not drive CP-AMPARs into synapses under the conditions of our experiments, there are conditions under which it can. For example, in the presence of rolipram, to inhibit breakdown of cAMP, a weak TBS effectively drives CP-AMPARs into synapses [16]. Also, neuromodulators, such as noradrenaline, enable a weak induction protocol to generate protein synthesis-dependent LTP [7, 25]. Finally, acute stress can drive CP-AMPARs into synapses and thereby facilitate LTP [24]. Indeed, the multiple ways of enhancing the synaptic insertion of CP-AMPARs likely has functional significance for neuromodulation.

In summary, our new observations are fully consistent with the role of CP-AMPARs in triggering heterosynaptic metaplasticity, since a sTBS, but not a wTBS, can drive the synaptic insertion of CP-AMPARs. However, we cannot discount the existence of other forms of heterosynaptic metaplasticity that may operate under different experimental conditions and may conform more closely with the predictions of the original STC hypothesis.

The requirement for synaptic activation post TBS

In set C, we explored whether stopping stimulation for 20 min following the sTBS was sufficient to prevent heterosynaptic metaplasticity. The logic behind this experiment is: if basal (low frequency) stimulation is required to activate the newly inserted CP-AMPARs to drive Ca2+ into the synapse [15] then stopping stimulation should have the equivalent effect on synaptic strength as applying a CP-AMPAR blocker, such as IEM-1460 [18]. This is indeed what we observed. For these experiments we also stopped stimulation following the delivery of the second TBS episode (apart from a few stimuli to monitor the level of potentiation) because these stimuli would be expected to activate the newly synaptically-inserted CP-AMPARs. The complete elimination of heterosynaptic metaplasticity by stopping stimulation re-enforces the essential role of CP-AMPARs in driving the process during the time window following the TBS protocol. In other words, by stopping stimulation during and following the sTBS, any membrane inserted CP-AMPARs are not activated to any appreciable extent and get passively removed. This results in a smaller LTP on the homosynaptic input, as reported previously [1620] and confirmed here, and no facilitation of LTP at the heterosynaptic input, as demonstrated here for the first time. At this juncture, we cannot dismiss the possibility that stopping stimulation has additional actions independent of CP-AMPARs that contribute to the effects that we observe. However, the simplest explanation is based solely around the activation of CP-AMPARs.

In set D, we asked whether stopping stimulation for 20 min following the wTBS was required for heterosynaptic metaplasticity. We again observed a significant effect, which is consistent with what we observed previously with IEM-1460. A partial inhibition of the STC effect was previously reported when an inhibitor of de novo protein synthesis is applied [2, 18]. Therefore, we can conclude that for the heterosynaptic metaplasticity to be optimally observed there has to be de novo protein synthesis triggered by CP-AMPARs that become synaptically available following the wTBS. The simplest explanation for this finding is that a sTBS not only drives CP-AMPARs homosynaptically but also drives CP-AMPARs heterosynaptically. In some cases, they may be inserted into the synapse where they mediate the small heterosynaptic LTP that is often observed when a protein synthesis-dependent LTP is induced. In other instances, they may dwell at perisynaptic sites awaiting a stimulus to drive them into the synapse, which the wTBS is able to do. In other words, they tag synapses for protein synthesis-dependent synaptic plasticity. Accordingly, a wTBS is able to drive these CP-AMPARs into the synapse where basal stimulation activates them to initiate highly localized protein synthesis. Appealing as this mechanism is, it is unlikely to be the entire explanation, since either stopping stimulation or the application of inhibitors of either CP-AMPARs or de novo protein synthesis only partially inhibits the heterosynaptic facilitation of LTP. At a subset of synapses, it is possible that the necessary activation of CP-AMPARs and de novo protein synthesis has already taken place and that the wTBS is just required to activate N-methyl-d-aspartate receptors (NMDARs) for a different necessary component of the induction process. Further work will be required to establish more precisely the underlying mechanisms of these two components of heterosynaptic metaplasticity.

Multiple forms of LTP and the roles of multiple subtypes of glutamate receptors

The role of glutamate receptors in LTP at the Schaffer collateral-commissural pathway is complex. Initially, it was shown that NMDARs are required [6]. Next it was found that mGluRs are sometimes necessary [5]. A role for CP-AMPARs was then identified in a KO mouse line, in which the GluA2 subunit was deleted [13]. Finally, a role for kainate receptors was found at these synapses at an early developmental stage [14]. LTP is similarly complex, comprising a family of overlapping processes that all require the activation of NMDARs under most circumstances. There is an initial potentiation, termed short-term potentiation (STP), that decays as the pathway is stimulated [23]. Then there are two forms of LTP that are stable over many hours (LTP1 and LTP2) that are distinguished on the basis of their requirement for de novo protein synthesis [4]. Longer lasting forms of LTP also require transcription (LTP3). Here we have focused on LTP1 and LTP2. We have provided additional evidence that CP-AMPARs confer heterosynaptic metaplasticity by priming synapses for enhanced LTP (i.e., converting LTP1 into LTP2 at the primed synapses). This process is therefore distinct from the role of mGluRs in LTP at these synapses where they are involved in a metaplasticity that is entirely homosynaptic in nature [5], though again involves de novo protein synthesis [21].

As originally hypothesised, the STC process is an ideal candidate synaptic mechanism for long-lasting associative memory [8]. Initially it was assumed that the de novo protein synthesis occurred at the soma and hence a tag was required to capture PRPs at synapses to enable their amplification. Subsequently, it was assumed that protein synthesis may occur locally [22]. Our mechanism [18] is based on local protein synthesis. Accordingly, the role of the tag is not to capture PRPs per se, but to mark a surround of synapses that are able to undergo enhanced LTP by enabling an LTP1-inducing stimulus to generate LTP2. This mechanism is entirely consistent with the notion of clustered synaptic plasticity [10]. A hypothetical scheme for the STC process appears in Additional file 1: Figure 1.

Concluding remarks

In summary, we can conclude that CP-AMPARs are an integral part of a form of heterosynaptic metaplasticity that is commonly referred to as the STC process. CP-AMPARs are inserted into the synapse in two stages, firstly via an NMDAR-triggered PKA-dependent insertion into perisynaptic sites and then by an NMDAR-triggered CaMKII-dependent movement into the synapse [19]. The former provides many opportunities for modulation via neurotransmitters and other factors that regulate cAMP levels in neurons. It also enables a heterosynaptic nature to synaptic plasticity, since CP-AMPARs may be inserted at sites outside of the activated synapses. The dwell time of CP-AMPARs on the plasma membrane is in the order of minutes so that they can associate signals that are appropriately spaced in time.

Methods

Experiments were performed as described in Park et al. [18]. Briefly, transverse hippocampal slices (400 μm) were prepared from male C57BL/6 mice (10–12 weeks of age) using a vibratome (Leica, VT1200S). The CA3 region was cut, with a scalpel blade, to suppress the upstream neuronal excitability, and the slices were transferred to an incubation chamber that contained the recording solution (artificial cerebrospinal fluid, ACSF, mM): 124 NaCl, 3 KCl, 26 NaHCO3, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 2 MgSO4, 2 CaCl2 and 10 d-glucose (carbonated with 95% O2 and 5% CO2). Slices were allowed to recover at 32–34 °C for 30 min, and then maintained at 26–28 °C for a minimum of 1 h before recordings were made. Hippocampal slices were continuously perfused at 3–4 mL/min with the oxygenated ACSF at 32 °C. Two bipolar stimulating electrodes were positioned in stratum radiatum on either side of the recording electrode at approximately the same distance from the cell body layer. Two independent Schaffer collateral-commissural pathways were stimulated alternately to obtain the evoked synaptic responses, each at a frequency of 0.1 Hz. The initial slope of the evoked field excitatory postsynaptic potential (fEPSP; V/s) was monitored and analysed using WinLTP [1]. Following a stable baseline period of at least 40 min, LTP was induced using theta-burst stimulation (TBS) delivered at the same basal stimulus intensity and pulse width (0.1 ms, constant voltage stimulator). An episode of TBS comprised bursts of 5 pulses at 100 Hz delivered at 5 Hz. The wTBS consisted of one episode of 3 bursts (i.e., 15 pulses in total). The sTBS comprised 3 episodes, each of 5 bursts, delivered with an inter-episode interval of 10 min (i.e., 75 pulses in total). Each experiment was performed on a slice obtained from a different mouse; therefore, n values denote both numbers of slices and mice used. Representative sample traces are an average of 5 consecutive responses, collected from typical experiments (stimulus artefacts were blanked for clarity). All four groups (A-D) were interleaved. Data are presented as mean ± SEM (standard error of the mean). Responses were normalised to the baseline prior to LTP induction, and data are expressed as % baseline. Statistical significance was assessed using one-way ANOVA with Bonferroni’s correction; the level of significance is denoted on the figures as follows: *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01 and ***p < 0.001.

Availability of data and materials

Key data supporting the conclusions of this article are included within the article. The datasets used and/or analysed are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

AMPARs:

α-Amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid receptor

CI-AMPARs:

Calcium-impermeable AMPARs

CP-AMPARs:

Calcium-permeable AMPARs

fEPSP:

Field excitatory postsynaptic potential

LTP:

Long-term potentiation

LTP1:

Protein synthesis-independent component of LTP

LTP2:

Protein synthesis-dependent component of LTP

LTP3:

Transcription-dependent component of LTP

NMDAR:

N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor

PRPs:

Plasticity-related proteins

sTBS:

Spaced TBS

STC:

Synaptic tag and capture

STP:

Short-term potentiation

TBS:

Theta burst stimulation

wTBS:

Weak TBS

References

  1. Anderson WW, Collingridge GL. Capabilities of the WinLTP data acquisition program extending beyond basic LTP experimental functions. J Neurosci Methods. 2007;162:346–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Barco A, Alarcon JM, Kandel ER. Expression of constitutively active CREB protein facilitates the late phase of long-term potentiation by enhancing synaptic capture. Cell. 2002;108:689–703.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Bliss TVP, Collingridge GL. A synaptic model of memory: long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Nature. 1993;361:31–9.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Bliss TVP, Collingridge GL, Morris RGM, Reymann KG. Long-term potentiation in the hippocampus: discovery, mechanisms and function. Neuroforum. 2018;24:A103-120. https://doi.org/10.1515/nf-2017-A059.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Bortolotto ZA, Bashir ZI, Davies CH, Collingridge GL. A molecular switch activated by metabotropic glutamate receptors regulates induction of long-term potentiation. Nature. 1994;368:740–3.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Collingridge GL, Kehl SJ, McLennan H. Excitatory amino acids in synaptic transmission in the Schaffer collateral-commissural pathway of the rat hippocampus. J Physiol. 1983;334:33–46.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Connor SA, Wang YT, Nguyen PV. Activation of {beta}-adrenergic receptors facilitates heterosynaptic translation-dependent long-term potentiation. J Physiol. 2011;589:4321–40.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Frey U, Morris RG. Synaptic tagging and long-term potentiation. Nature. 1997;385:533–6.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Frey U, Morris RG. Weak before strong: dissociating synaptic tagging and plasticity-factor accounts of late-LTP. Neuropharmacology. 1998;37:545–52.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Govindarajan A, Israely I, Huang S-Y, Tonegawa S. The dendritic branch is the preferred integrative unit for protein synthesis-dependent LTP. Neuron. 2011;69:132–46.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Huang YY, Kandel ER. Recruitment of long-lasting and protein kinase A-dependent long-term potentiation in the CA1 region of hippocampus requires repeated tetanization. Learn Mem. 1994;1:74–82.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Hulme SR, Jones OD, Raymond CR, Sah P, Abraham WC. Mechanisms of heterosynaptic metaplasticity. Philos Trans R Soc B. 2014;369:20130148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Jia Z, Agopyan N, Miu P, Xiong Z, Henderson J, Gerlai R, Taverna FA, Velumian A, MacDonald J, Carlen P, Abramow-Newerly W, Roder J. Enhanced LTP in mice deficient in the AMPA receptor GluR2. Neuron. 1996;17:945–56.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Lauri SE, Vesikansa A, Segerstrale M, Collingridge GL, Isaac JTR, Taira T. Functional maturation of CA1 synapses involves activity-dependent loss of tonic kainate receptor-mediated inhibition of glutamate release. Neuron. 2006;50:415–29.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Morita D, Rah J-C, Isaac JTR. Incorporation of inwardly rectifying AMPA receptors at silent synapses during hippocampal long-term potentiation. Philos Trans R Soc B. 2014;369:20130156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Park P, Sanderson TM, Amici M, Choi S-L, Bortolotto ZA, Zhuo M, Kaang B-K, Collingridge GL. Calcium-permeable AMPA receptors mediate the induction of the protein kinase A-dependent component of long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. J Neurosci. 2016;36:622–31.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Park P, Kang H, Sanderson TM, Bortolotto ZA, Georgiou J, Zhuo M, Kaang B-K, Collingridge GL. The role of calcium-permeable AMPARs in long-term potentiation at principal neurons in the rodent hippocampus. Front Synaptic Neurosci. 2018;10:42.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Park P, Kang H, Sanderson TM, Bortolotto ZA, Georgiou J, Zhuo M, Kaang B-K, Collingridge GL. On the role of calcium-permeable AMPARs in long-term potentiation and synaptic tagging in the rodent hippocampus. Front Synaptic Neurosci. 2019;11:4598.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Park P, Georgiou J, Sanderson TM, Ko K-H, Kang H, Kim J-I, Bradley CA, Bortolotto ZA, Zhuo M, Kaang B-K, Collingridge GL. PKA drives an increase in AMPA receptor unitary conductance during LTP in the hippocampus. Nat Commun. 2021;12(1):413. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-20523-3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Plant K, Pelkey KA, Bortolotto ZA, Morita D, Terashima A, McBain CJ, Collingridge GL, Isaac JTR. Transient incorporation of native GluR2-lacking AMPA receptors during hippocampal long-term potentiation. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9:602–4.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Raymond CR, Thompson VL, Tate WP, Abraham WC. Metabotropic glutamate receptors trigger homosynaptic protein synthesis to prolong long-term potentiation. J Neurosci. 2000;20:969–76.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. Redondo RL, Morris RGM. Making memories last: the synaptic tagging and capture hypothesis. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011;12:17–30.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. Volianskis A, Jensen MS. Transient and sustained types of long-term potentiation in the CA1 area of the rat hippocampus. J Physiol. 2003;550:459–92.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Whitehead G, Jo J, Hogg EL, Piers T, Kim D-H, Seaton G, Seok H, Bru-Mercier G, Son GH, Regan P, Hildebrandt L, Waite E, Kim B-C, Kerrigan TL, Kim K, Whitcomb DJ, Collingridge GL, Lightman SL, Cho K. Acute stress causes rapid synaptic insertion of Ca2+ -permeable AMPA receptors to facilitate long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Brain. 2013;136:3753–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Zhang M, Patriarchi T, Stein IS, Qian H, Matt L, Nguyen M, Xiang YK, Hell JW. Adenylyl cyclase anchoring by a kinase anchor protein AKAP5 (AKAP79/150) is important for postsynaptic β-adrenergic signaling. J Biol Chem. 2013;288:17918–31.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

This work was supported by the CIHR (GLC), the EJLB-CIHR Michael Smith Chair in Neurosciences and Mental Health, Canada Research Chair, and Canadian Institute for Health Research operating Grants (CIHR66975 and 84256) (MZ) and the National Honor Scientist Program of the National Research Foundation funded by the South Korea Government (B-KK). This work was also supported by the Brain Canada Foundation through the Canada Brain Research Fund, with the financial support of Health Canada.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

PP and HK performed the experiments, analysis and prepared the manuscript. JG, MZ, and B-KK contributed to the work and co-wrote the manuscript. GC designed the studies and co-wrote the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Graham L. Collingridge.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Research was carried out according to the UK Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act of 1986 and approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees at the University of Bristol and Seoul National University.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial or non-financial interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1: Figure 1.

Hypothetical scheme for heterosynaptic metaplasticity.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Park, P., Kang, H., Georgiou, J. et al. Further evidence that CP-AMPARs are critically involved in synaptic tag and capture at hippocampal CA1 synapses. Mol Brain 14, 26 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13041-021-00737-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13041-021-00737-2

Keywords